Skip to main content

What have Caribbean lawyers got to do with the danger of falling out of bed?

Turns out, not much. The statistics for people who died falling out of their bed correlate almost perfectly (0.96) with the number of laywers in Peurto Rico. This stat comes from the rather wonderful tylervigen.com which collects many (many, many) such examples.

Some time spent browsing that site gets the point across that correlation does not imply causation. This means that even if two things vary together, you can't assume they are linked. You definitely can't assume that one is causing the other. It's a logical fallacy known as cum hoc ergo propter hoc, or "With this, therefore because of this".

This fallacy is immensely important to psychology research. The traditional scientific method involves testing causality by changing a single, "independent" variable, then measuring what happens to another variable. If the second, "dependent" variable changes too, then there is a causal relationship between them. Changes in the independent variable create changes in the dependent variable.

This works for some psychology experiments. You can measure relatively simple behaviours, modify some small aspect of a test, and then measure again. An example of this might be priming, a psychology favourite in which people are shown something for a brief period, which makes their subsequent behaviour different to having been shown nothing. These effects are often subtle, and small variations in individual behaviour might mask them. To overcome this, you repeat the experiment with many people, increasing your sample size.

Not all psychology studies are as amenable to a simple lab experiment as this, though. What if you only have one person who has a particular condition you want to study? You can't just have them do the same thing over and over again. Worse, what if the kind of outcome you're looking for defies simple measurement? Or it could entirely unethical, or downright impossible, to replicate the conditions you're studying? Then all you're left with is correlation.

Correlation tells you two things vary together, but it doesn't tell you anything about which causes the other. There are a number of different possibilities, that x causes y, that y causes x, or that there's a third thing, z, that affects both x and y together. There's another even more interesting scenario in that x and y might have a transactional relationship. If this true, when x changes, y changes, which in return changes x, and so on in a spiral.

One of the most awkward examples of this for psychology is the way that psychological knowledge itself can change behaviour. If you take knowledge in physics, for example, when you come up with a new theory and publish, whatever phenomenon you're studying just carries on behaving as it did before. In psychology, however, a new theory might well change the very behaviour it covers.

This post has been pretty theory-heavy, but it does have a major practical application to add to your critical-thinking toolbox. You should treat any research which just shows a correlation with extreme caution. It's one of the tabloid press' favourite headline generators, which leads to all sort of crazy claims. Take a look through today's paper. The Mail Online is probably the best hunting ground for this kind of error, but all papers do it. Look for an article which claims that x causes y, pick it apart and see if it's just correlation. If the original study it's based on just suggests x and y vary together, then you've got your logically fallacy. Let me know what you've found by posting in the comments.

This problem can come up in an engineering context, too. If things in the environment change, and so does user behaviour, it can be easy to see a causal link between the two. This can lead you astray because there are many reasons for users to change behaviour, not just that there was an alteration to some aspect of the product people use. One of the best ways to address this is to use A-B testing, so that you run two different versions (e.g. to different website pages) alongside each other and measure the outcomes of both.

By using this approach and the scientific method you can separate out cause and effect, whilst protecting yourself from seeing relationships between factors that simply aren't there.


Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Starting at the start - why write a blog?

Why start a blog on Pychology for IT Pros (and other technical people)? Whilst there's a huge amount of information tailored to traditional business, and an increasing body of work around startups and their associated culture, there is very little that directly relates to the day-to-day working life of technical people and what they do. This is a major omission; whilst technical people may be different in that part of the job involves dealing with machines, a huge part of the work is with, for, and mediated by, other people. There's a good argument that the role of  IT people is to form the bridge between computer systems and the rest of the organisation. The aim of this blog is to use evidence from psychology, neuroscience and other related fields to give you insight into how you can do your job more effectively, and be happier whilst doing so. You may well be wondering if reading this blog will be worth your time, aside from the absence of anyone else writing on the the t...

Why work?

This is the first of a series of post on happiness at work. Happiness is one of the most valuable practical outcomes of applying psychology, and doing so at work can be transformative. We typically spend 40 hours a week working, which is a very long time to be doing something which makes us miserable. Few people I've met had really made a point of pursuing happiness at work. It's not a widely held expectation that we should enjoy our jobs, and perhaps even the opposite is true, that there is some moral value in spending time doing something you find unpleasant. This internalisation of the Protestant Work Ethic has been, perhaps surprisingly, escaped the Danes. They have word for happiness at work,  Arbejdsglæde,  and both expect the workplace to allow them to be happy, and make decisions in support of that. This has considerable benefits, including a higher per-capita GDP than the UK. Before I get stuck in, it's important to define what I mean by happiness here. What I...

How to change things

In the past few posts I've covered quite a few potential sources of dissatisfaction at work. You might have found yourself in a job which sits poorly with your personal identity , or you may have found yourself in a workplace which lacks sufficient trust . This post is focused on some methods with a good psychological backing to help you change yourself so you can get closer to the things you want. I'm focusing on self-change here as it's perhaps the most fundamental thing you can do to address an unhappy situation, and has the most profound effects. There's a lot of resonance in this topic for me personally, and this blog is a part of my own change process. First, I'm going to discuss approaches that make change more likely, then I'll move on to techniques for determining what kind of positive change you really want to make. Making change more likely Much like actively designing a computer system is far better than letting circumstance design it for you, pla...